Abstract
Humour is often employed as a coping mechanism, with therapeutic effects on those producing and receiving it (Christopher 2015; Samson & Gross 2012). This buffering effect of humour might explain why, at the time of an international pandemic like Covid-19, human beings, independently of their cultural origin, have resorted to humour as a means of alleviating uncertainty and fear, and of enhancing feelings of connection and bonding with others. The proliferation of Covid-related humour has also led to a wide range of studies, with special attention to memes. However, contrastive studies are more limited, especially those comparing very different languages and cultural realities such as the Chinese, the Czech and the Spanish ones. This paper aims to redress this imbalance by analysing a corpus of 300 Covid-memes (100 memes per language). More specifically, we intend to answer the following questions: (i) what dimension(s) of humour are predominant in each language? (ii) what actors do the memes in the three countries target? and (iii) to what extent can these preferences relate to cultural differences/similarities? Applying a mixed-method approach, results show that there seems to be a global preference for affiliative humour while aggressive (and self-deprecating) humour appears to be more culturally bound, with a higher frequency in the Czech and Spanish datasets in contrast to the Chinese one. Likewise, the Czech and Spanish dataset share a significantly higher number of common frames, which might be pointing to a more European, Western type of humour in comparison to the Chinese approach (Jiang et al. 2019).
References
Balarezo-López, G. (2020). ‘Reír para no sufrir: la pandemia del Coronavirus a través del humor de los memes en el Whatsapp’ [Laughing not to suffer: Coronavirus pandemic through meme humour on WhatsApp]. Paideia XXI 10 (2), pp. 555-572.
Besser, A. & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2011). ‘Pathological forms of narcissism and perceived stress during the transition to the university: the mediating role of humor styles’. International Journal of Stress Management 18, pp. 197-221.
Cancelas-Ouviña, L.-P. (2021). ‘Humor in times of COVID-19 in Spain: viewing coronavirus through memes disseminated via WhatsApp’. Frontiers in Psychology 12.
Chen, Y. C. (2001). ‘Chinese values, health and nursing’. Journal of Advanced Nursing 36 (2), pp. 270-273.
Cohen, J. (1960). ‘A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales’. Educational and Psychological Measurement 20, pp. 37–46.
Chiodo, C. P., Broughton, K. K. & Michalski, M. P. (2020). ‘Caution: wit and humor during the COVID-19 pandemic’. Foot & Ankle International 41 (6), pp. 763–764.
Chłopicki, W., & Brzozowska, D. (2021). ‘Sophisticated humor against COVID-19: the Polish case’. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 34 (2), pp. 201-227.
Christopher, S. (2015). ‘An introduction to black humour as a coping mechanism for student paramedics’. Journal of Paramedic Practice 7 (12), pp. 610-617.
Damianakis, T. & Marziali, E. (2011). ‘Community-dwelling older adults' contextual experiencing of humour’. Ageing and Society 31, pp. 110-124.
Dean, J. (2019). ‘Sorted for memes and gifs: visual media and everyday digital politics’. Political Studies Review 17 (3), pp. 255–266.
Del Río, C. M. (2013). ‘Can ethnic humour appreciation be influenced by political reasons?’ The European Journal of Humour Research 1 (2), pp. 24-42.
Dynel, M. (2013). Developments in Linguistic Humour Theory. Topics in Humor Research, Vol. 1. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ferguson, M. A., & Ford, T. E. (2008). ‘Disparagement humor: a theoretical and empirical review of psychoanalytic, superiority, and social identity theories’. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 21 (3), pp. 283–312.
Flecha O. J. A., et al. (2021). ‘Analysis of the use of memes as an exponent of collective coping during COVID-19 in Puerto Rico’. Media International Australia 178 (1), pp. 168-181.
Flett, J. A. M., Lie, C., Riordan, B. C., Thompson, L. M., Conner, T. S. & Hayne, H. (2017). ‘Sharpen your pencils: preliminary evidence that adult coloring reduces depressive symptoms and anxiety’. Creativity Research Journal 29 (4), pp. 409-416.
Frankl, V. E. (2015). Man's Search for Meaning. Boston: Beacon Press.
Geeraerts, D. (2008). Cognitive Linguistics: Basic Readings. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter Mouton.
Giskin, H. & Walsh, B. S. (eds.). (2001). An Introduction to Chinese Culture through the Family. Albany, NY: Suny Press.
Hallgren K. A. (2012). ‘Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: an overview and tutorial’. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology 8 (1), pp. 23–34.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations across Cultures. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Holý, L. (2010). Malý Český Člověk a Skvělý Český Národ: Národní Identita a Postkomunistická Transformace Společnosti [Small Czech Person and Great Czech Nation: National Identity and Post-communist Transformation of the Society]. Praha: Sociologické nakladatelství.
Huang, F., Teo, T., Sánchez-Prieto, J. C., García-Peñalvo, F. J. & Olmos-Migueláñez, S. (2019). ‘Cultural values and technology adoption: a model comparison with university teachers from China and Spain’. Computers & Education 133, pp. 69-81.
Jacques-Aviñó, C., López-Jiménez, T., Medina, L., Rodríguez, I., Queiroga, A, Duarte-Salles, T., de Bont, J. & Berenguera, A. (2021). ‘El impacto psicosocial del confinamiento domiciliario por la pandèmia de la COVID-19 en España’ [Psychosocial impact of home confinement due to the COVID-19 pandemic in Spain]. Institut Universitari per a la recerca a l'Atenció Primària de Salut Jordi Gol i Gurina (IDIAPJGol).
Jensen, M. S., Neumayer, C. & Rossi, L. (2020). ‘‘Brussels will land on its feet like a cat’: motivations for memefying #Brusselslockdown’. Information, Communication & Society 23 (1), pp. 59–75.
Jiang, T., Li, H. & Hou, Y. (2019). ‘Cultural differences in humor perception, usage, and implications’. Frontiers in Psychology 10, 123.
Kolaříková, V. (2020). ‘Czech national identity and the elements through which is constructed’. Czech-Polish Historical and Pedagogical Journal 12 (2), pp. 66–96.
Kuipers, G. (2009). ‘Humor styles and symbolic boundaries’. Journal of Literary Theory 3 (2), pp. 219-239.
Kulhánek, A. (2021, February 11). ‘WHO zkoumala, proč je v Česku tolik nakažených. Jste bezohlední a šíříte virus, vzkazuje prostřednictvím výzkumu’ [WHO investigated why there are so many infected people in the Czech Republic. You are careless and spread the virus, it says through the research]. Mix24.cz. Retrieved April 20, 2022 from: https://www.mix24.cz/who-zkoumala-proc-je-v-cesku-tolik-nakazenych-jste-bezohledni-a-sirite-virus-vzkazuje-prostrednictvim-vyzkumu/
Lee, S. L. (2018). ‘Why color mandalas? A study of anxiety-reducing mechanisms’. Art Therapy 35 (1), pp. 35-41.
Light, R. J. (1971) ‘Measures of response agreement for qualitative data: Some generalizations and alternatives’. Psychological Bulletin 76 (5), pp. 365–377.
Littrell, R. F. (2007). ‘Influences on employee preferences for empowerment practices by the “ideal manager” in China’. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (1), pp. 87-110.
Löblová, O. (2020, November 9). ‘Coronavirus: what went wrong in the Czech Republic?’. The Conversation. Retrieved April 20, 2022 from: https://theconversation.com/coronavirus-what-went-wrong-in-the-czech-republic-149505
Marcus, O. R. & Singer, M. (2017). ‘Loving Ebola-chan: Internet memes in an epidemic’. Media, Culture & Society 39 (3), pp. 341-356.
Martin, R. A., et al. (2003). ‘Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: development of the Humor Styles Questionnaire’. Journal of Research in Personality 37 (1), pp. 48-75.
Mayer, C. H., & Mayer, L. J. (2021). ‘Humour as a coping strategy for employees in remote workspaces during Covid-19’, in Vanderheiden, E. & Mayer, C. H. (eds.), The Palgrave Handbook of Humour Research, Cham: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 289-307.
Mervis, C. B. & Rosch, E. (1981). ‘Categorization of natural objects’. Annual Review of Psychology 32, pp. 89–115.
Meyer, J. (2000). ‘Humor as a double‐edged sword: four functions of humor in communication’. Communication Theory 10, pp. 310-331.
Miczo, N. & Averbeck, J. & Mariani, T. (2009). ‘Affiliative and aggressive humor, attachment dimensions, and interaction goals’. Communication Studies 60, pp. 443-459.
Miczo, N. & Welter, R. E. (2006). ‘Aggressive and affiliative humor: relationships to aspects of intercultural communication’. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research 35 (1), pp. 61-77.
Mir, M. & Cots, J. M. (2019). ‘The use of humor in Spanish and English compliment responses: a cross-cultural analysis’. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 32 (3), pp. 393-416.
Makhortykh, M. & González Aguilar, J. M. (2020). ‘Memory, politics and emotions: internet memes and protests in Venezuela and Ukraine’. Continuum 34 (3), pp. 342-362.
Mpofu, S. (ed.). (2021). Digital Humour in the Covid-19 Pandemic Perspectives from the Global South. London: Palgrave.
Norstrom, R. & Sarna, P. (2021). ‘Internet memes in Covid-19 lockdown times in Poland’. Comunicar: Media Education Research Journal 29 (67), pp. 69-79. https://doi.org/10.3916/C67-2021-06.
Novinky. cz. (2021, April 12). ‘Nová statistika: Česko má nejvíc obětí koronaviru na obyvatele na světě’[New statistics: Czech Republic has the most victims of coronavirus in the world]. Retrieved April 20, 2022 from: https://www.novinky.cz/koronavirus/clanek/nova-statistika-cesko-ma-nejvic-obeti-koronaviru-na-obyvatele-na-svete-40356693
Olson, M. L. et al. (2005) ‘Rumination and dysphoria: the buffering role of adaptive forms of
humor’. Personality and Individual Differences 39 (8), pp. 1419-1428.
Piwowarczyk, K. (2019). ‘Chinese and Western comedy–an introduction into cross-cultural humour research between Taiwan, China and the West’. Nowa Polityka Wschodnia 20 (1), pp. 23-39.
Ponton, D. & Mantello, P. (2021). ‘Virality, contagion and public discourse. The role of memes as prophylaxis and catharsis in an age of crisis’. Rhetoric and Communication 46, pp. 44-63.
Rosch, E. (1978) ‘Principles of categorisation’, in Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B. B. (eds.), Cognition and Categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 27-48.
Samson, A.C. & Gross, J.J.. (2012). ‘Humour as emotion regulation: the differential consequences of negative versus positive humour’. Cognition & Emotion 26 (2), pp. 375-384.
Saroglou, V. & Scariot, C. (2002). ‘Humor styles questionnaire: personality and educational correlates in Belgian high school and college students’. European Journal of Personality 16 (1), pp. 43–54.
Shifman, L. (2014). Memes in Digital Culture. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press
Sola-Morales, S. (2020). ‘Humor en tiempos de pandemia. Análisis de memes digitales sobre la COVID-19’ [Humor in pandemic times. Analysis of digital memes about the COVID-19]. Zer: Revista De Estudios De Comunicación = Komunikazio Ikasketen Aldizkaria, 25 (49), pp. 33-58. https://doi.org/10.1387/zer.21817
Stamkou, E. et al. (2019). ‘Cultural collectivism and tightness moderate responses to norm violators: effects on power perception, moral emotions, and leader support’. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45 (6), pp. 947-964.
Stefani, C. (2020). ‘Humor during pandemic in Romania on Facebook’. Culture e Studi del Sociale-CuSSoc 5 (1), pp. 323-334.
Šubrt, J., Vinopal, J. et al. (2013). Historické Vědomí Obyvatel České Republiky Perspektivou Sociologického Výzkumu. [Historic Awareness of the Citizens of the Czech Republic through the Perspective of Sociological Research] Praha: Karolinum.
Twitter.com/CoronaVid19. [Last retrieved July 2022]
Uskul, A. K., Oyserman, D. & Schwarz, N. (2010). ‘Cultural emphasis on honor, modesty, or self-enhancement: implications for the survey-response process’, in Harkness, J. A., et al. (eds.), Survey Methods in Multinational, Multiregional, and Multicultural Contexts. John Wiley & Sons, pp. 191-201.
Vinokurova, V. (2021). ‘Memes: learning, bonding, and emotional support in times of COVID-19’. Russian Language Journal 71 (2), pp. 271-288.
Xiong, M., Wang, F. & Cai, R. (2018). ‘Development and validation of the Chinese Modesty Scale (CMS)’. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 2014.
Young, L. C. (1988). ‘Regional stereotypes in China’. Chinese Studies in History 21 (4), pp. 32-57.
Yus, F. (2021). Smartphone Communication: Interactions in the App Ecosystem. Abingdon: Routledge.
Vlachová, K. & Řeháková, B. (2009). ‘Identity of non-self-evident nation: Czech national identity after the break-up of Czechoslovakia and before accession to the European Union’. Nations and Nationalism 15 (2), pp. 254–279.
Walkinshaw, I., Mitchell, N. & Subhan, S. (2019). ‘Self-denigration as a relational strategy in lingua franca talk: Asian English speakers’. Journal of Pragmatics 139, pp. 40-51.
Wiggins, B. E. (2016). ‘Crimea River: directionality in memes from the Russia-Ukraine conflict’. International Journal of Communication 10 (35), pp. 451–485.
World Health Organisation (WHO). (2020). Situation Report - 71. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19). Available at https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/situation-report---71.
Wutz, I. (2016). ‘Brexit and online political activism: on vox populism, slacktivism and online intertextuality’. Digital Magazine. Retrieved from https://www.diggitmagazine.com/papers/brexit-and-online-political-activism.
Ziv, A. 1984. Personality and Sense of Humour. New York: Springer.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Copyright (c) 2022 The European Journal of Humour Research